• Custom Writing Services
  • How to Write a Research Paper
  • Research Paper Topics
  • Research Paper Examples
  • Order

Communication

iResearchNet

Custom Writing Services

Custom Writing Services
Communication » Media » History of Media Effects

History of Media Effects




The established history of media effects research is characterized by a series of phases marked by fundamental paradigm shifts (see McQuail 1977, 72 –74; 2005, 457– 462; Lowery & DeFleur 1983, 22 –29; Severin & Tankard 2001, 262 –268; Baran & Davis 2006, 8 –17). Each of these phases is associated with particular concepts, researchers, studies, and historical circumstances that influenced ideological development regarding media effects.

The Four Phases Of Media Effects Paradigms

The first phase, from World War I to the end of the 1930s, was characterized by the assumption that the effects of the media on the population would be exceedingly strong. The media were credited with an almost limitless omnipotence in their ability to shape opinion and belief, to change life habits, and to mold audience behavior more or less according to the will of their controllers (McQuail 2005, 458). The power of media messages over unsuspecting audiences was described in drastic terms: the mass media supposedly fired messages like dangerous bullets, or shot messages into the audience like strong drugs pushed through hypodermic needles. These descriptions gave rise to the “hypodermicneedle concept” (Berlo 1960, 27), the “magic bullet theory” (Schramm 1973, 243), and the “transmission belt theory” (DeFleur & Ball-Rokeach 1982, 161). Instinct psychology and the theory of mass society were interpreted to show that people in urbanized and industrialized society were rootless, alienated, and inherently susceptible to manipulation. As a result, they were defenseless against and at the mercy of the capricious stimuli of the media – particularly as early ideas maintained that the mass media were run primarily by people and organizations that were deliberately trying to exert a targeted influence upon recipients.




The second phase of the standard history lasted approximately from the end of the 1930s to the end of the 1960s and was distinguished by the assumption that the media were largely not influential. The research group of Paul F. Lazarsfeld ushered in the deconstruction of the bullet theory. The results of their empirical, social-scientific election study, The people’s choice (1944), moved interest away from what the media did to people and toward what people did with the media. Rather than seeing a society of fragmented individuals receiving all-powerful messages from the mass media, the view shifted to one of a society of individuals who interacted within groups and thus limited the effects of media messages. Early on, Lazarsfeld et al. (1944) defined all three key concepts that Joseph T. Klapper (1960) later united and used as the basis of his limited effects theory. These three concepts also characterized the second phase of effects research. They state that: (1) people use selective exposure and selective perception to protect themselves from media influences, accepting almost exclusively only such information as corresponds to their previously established attitudes; (2) opinion leaders initiate a two-step flow of communication by absorbing ideas and arguments from the mass media and then communicating these – transformed – ideas to less active individuals; (3) social group formation enhances the role that interpersonal communication plays in protecting an individual member from a change of opinion, as members do not wish to lose membership in their relational group.

The third phase, from the end of the 1960s through the end of the 1970s, was characterized by the rediscovery of strong media effects. According to standard media effects history, an essay by Elisabeth Noelle-Neumann entitled “Return to the concept of powerful mass media” (1973) may be considered to have set the program for the movement into the third phase (see Severin & Tankard 2001, 264; McQuail 2005, 460). A number of highly regarded studies showed that it was possible for the media to overcome some selectivity processes in a television-saturated environment. Near the end of the 1940s Herbert Hyman and Paul Sheatsly (1947) published a study in Public Opinion Quarterly entitled “Some reasons why information campaigns fail”; then, a quarter of a century later, Harold Mendelsohn (1973) used the same forum to proclaim the exact opposite: “Some reasons why information campaigns can succeed.” Three distinct features are attributed to this phase: more sophisticated methods of analysis, more specific hypotheses, and more highly differentiated theoretical approaches. Thus, survey data and content analysis data could be combined long-term with the help of time-series analyses or panel design studies. In addition, effects research since that time has been less focused on crude changes in attitude or behavior, and more interested in subtle changes in our perception of the world.

The fourth phase of the standard media effects history extends through to the present time and is characterized by “negotiated” or “transactional” effects (McQuail 2005, 461). Now the central premise maintains that the media exert their greatest influence when they become involved in the process of constructing sense and meaning. Typical theories connected with this new approach are social constructivism, cultivation theory, framing, and information processing theories. McQuail considers research in this vein to be driven by two insights:

First, media “construct” social formations and even history itself by framing images of reality (in fiction as well as news) in predictable and patterned ways. Second, people in audiences construct for themselves their own view of social reality and their place in it, in interaction with the symbolic constructions offered by the media. The approach allows both for the power of media and for the power of people to choose, with a terrain of continuous negotiation in between, as it were. (McQuail 2005, 461)

Challenges To The Four-Phases Model

The oversimplified account of the received view of media effects history has been criticized harshly in recent times. Lang and Lang (1993, 93) called the alleged sequence of phases unrealistic “paradoxes” that feigned contradictions that had never existed. Instead, they maintained that “a considerable continuity” had been prevalent in the research community over the decades (Lang & Lang 1981, 662). Even proponents of the phase model felt forced to play down its heuristic value as time progressed (see McQuail 2005, 460). It seems that the model was able to establish itself so firmly because it offered a clear summary of a complex developmental process. However, current thought considers it evident that, in every period, studies could be identified that indicated limited or powerful effects – depending on what operationalizations, conceptualizations, definitions, measurements, designs, and variables were used.

Likewise, careful reanalyses of research literature from the first phase of effects studies show that “few, if any, reputable social scientists in the pre-World War II era . . . worked with what was later described as the hypodermic needle model” (Lang & Lang 1981, 655). Even the empirical findings from the second phase, upon closer inspection, show no justification for an overall verdict of media impotence (Lang & Lang 1981, 659). Instead, numerous studies from that time indicating the presence of media effects can be identified. Due to the prevailing opinions of the time, however, no notice was taken of these findings. Two main factors explain the successful run enjoyed by the “minimal effects myth”: first, there was an exaggerated concentration of a limited range of effect types (especially short-term attitude change during election campaigns); second, there was a one-sided and inappropriate interpretation of the results of three key studies, which further secondary literature adopted without additional review.

In the first of these key studies, Lazarsfeld et al.’s The people’s choice (1944), the data in no way unequivocally supported both central investigative findings – the importance of interpersonal communication (“two-step flow”) and of reinforcement instead of chance (“minimal effects”). In spite of the fact that 61 percent of the interviewees named newspaper (23 percent) and radio (38 percent) as their “most important sources” of information during the election, the authors alleged that it is not the media but people who can move other people (although less than one fourth cited a personal source as important). Moreover, in spite of the fact that 8 percent of those questioned did indeed alter their voting decision because of media influences, the authors interpreted this as evidence for a lack of effect (see Chaffee & Hochheimer 1985, 273, 279). Not only is 8 percent a considerable change, it should also be noted that the authors were concerned only with voting intention and ignored other possible political effects where media impact might have been even greater.

In the second key study, Personal influence (1955) by Katz and Lazarsfeld, an inappropriate claim was made to the effect that all previous effects research had been based on the following framework: “that of the omnipotent media, on the one hand, sending forth the message, and the atomized masses, on the other, waiting to receive it – and nothing in between” (1955, 20). In retrospect, Katz (1987, S35) admitted that early empirical communications research seems not to have based its efforts on the idea outlined in 1955, which propounded an omnipotent media and the stimulus–response model arising from this assumption. Nevertheless, Katz and Lazarsfeld’s book created a mythos that has definitively influenced the history of this field even up to today (see Delia 1987, 65 – 66).

Biased Perceptions Of Media Effects

From Klapper’s synopsis The effects of mass communication (1960), the third key work of that era, secondary literature adopted primarily those conclusions that pointed to minimal effects, failing to subject these inferences to review. However, Klapper did also clearly define conditions under which the media could develop strong effects. Even so, since he provided only very few pieces of evidence and examples for these in his one-sided presentation, they made no impression on the readers of the time or on later generations of research (see Perse 2001, 25). In addition, Klapper worked as director of social research for CBS, one of the largest media corporations in the United States, and media companies were uninterested in evidence supporting the strength of the media. Quite the contrary: they were interested in evidence proving the insignificance of media effects and used Klapper’s book to argue against regulation (Perse 2001, 28).

The apparent change of mind leading to the rediscovery of strong effects may also be better explained by factors outside of, rather than within, the research world. The rapid spread of television during the 1960s and 1970s lent a political dimension to the field of effects research. Influential commercial and political forces increasingly accused the media of failing to respect these entities’ interests and, consequently, of distortion. Such allegations drew heightened public attention to the effectiveness of the media.

Today, a growing number of scholars agree that the established standard history of the field is misleading because it tends to ignore those findings that do not fit neatly into the stage-by-stage scenario. Many authors (e.g., Lang & Lang 1981; Chaffee & Hochheimer 1985; McLeod et al. 1991; Wartella 1996; Bryant & Thompson 2002) have thus concluded that the development of mass media effects research did not move in pendulum swings from “all-powerful” to “limited” to “rediscovered powerful” to “negotiated” effects. Bryant and Thompson (2002, 42, 58) argue that the body of media effects research from the beginning showed overwhelming evidence for significant effects. Thus, the sum total of all these considerations yields the conclusion that the history of media effects research still waits to be written (see Wartella 1996, 179).

References:

  1. Baran, S. J., & Davis, D. K. (2006). Mass communication theory, 4th edn. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
  2. Berlo, D. K. (1960). The process of communication. An introduction to theory and practice. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
  3. Bryant, J., & Thompson, S. (2002). Fundamentals of media effects. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
  4. Chaffee, S. H., & Hochheimer, J. L. (1985). The beginnings of political communication research in the United States: Origins of the “limited effects” model. In E. M. Rogers & F. Balle (eds.), The media revolution in America and in Western Europe. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 267–296.
  5. DeFleur, M. L., & Ball-Rokeach, S. J. (1982). Theories of mass communication. New York: Longman.
  6. Delia, J. G. (1987). Communication research: A history. In C. R. Berger & S. H. Chaffee (eds.), Handbook of communication science. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, pp. 20 – 98.
  7. Hyman, H. H., & Sheatsly, P. B. (1947). Some reasons why information campaigns fail. Public Opinion Quarterly, 11, 412 – 423.
  8. Katz, E. (1987). Communication research since Lazarsfeld. Public Opinion Quarterly, 51 (special issue), S25 –S45.
  9. Katz, E., & Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1955). Personal influence. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
  10. Klapper, J. T. (1960). The effects of mass communication. New York: Free Press.
  11. Lang, G. E., & Lang, K. (1981). Mass communication and public opinion: Strategies for research. In M. Rosenberg & R. H. Turner (eds.), Social psychology: Sociological perspectives. New York: Basic Books, pp. 653 – 682.
  12. Lang, G. E., & Lang, K. (1993). Perspectives on communication. Journal of Communication, 43, 92– 99.
  13. Lazarsfeld, P. F., Berelson, B., & Gaudet, H. (1944). The people’s choice. How the voter makes up his mind in a presidential election. New York: Columbia University Press.
  14. Lowery, S. A., & DeFleur, M. L. (1983). Milestones of mass communication research: Media effects. White Plains: Longman.
  15. McLeod, J. M., Kosicki, G. M., & Pan, Z. (1991). On understanding and misunderstanding media effects. In J. Curran, M. Gurevitch, & J. Woollacott (eds.), Mass media and society. London: Edward Arnold, pp. 235 –266.
  16. McQuail, D. (1977). The influence and effects of mass media. In J. Curran, M. Gurevitch, & J. Woollacott (eds.), Mass communication and society. London: Edward Arnold, pp. 70 – 94.
  17. McQuail, D. (2005). McQuail’s mass communication theory, 5th edn. London: Sage.
  18. Mendelsohn, H. (1973). Some reasons why information campaigns can succeed. Public Opinion Quarterly, 37, 50 – 61.
  19. Noelle-Neumann, E. (1973). Return to the concept of powerful mass media. Studies of Broadcasting, 9, 66 –112.
  20. Perse, E. (2001). Media effects and society. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  21. Schramm, W. (1973). Men, messages, and media. A look at human communication. New York: Harper and Row.
  22. Severin, W. J., & Tankard, J. W. (2001). Communication theories: Origins, methods, and uses in the mass media, 4th edn. New York: Longman.
  23. Wartella, E. (1996). The history reconsidered. In E. E. Davis & E. Wartella (eds.), American communication research: The remembered history. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 169 –180.




Communication Research

Communication Research

  • Media
    • Media Economics
    • Media Effects
    • Media History
    • Media Production and Content
    • Media Systems
    • Media and Perceptions of Reality
    • Excitation Transfer Theory
    • Effects Of Exemplification And Exemplars
    • Economics of Advertising
    • Antitrust Regulation
    • Audience Commodity
    • Brands
    • Circulation
    • Commercialization of the Media
    • Competition in Media Systems
    • Concentration in Media Systems
    • Consolidation of Media Markets
    • Consumers in Media Markets
    • Cost and Revenue Structures in the Media
    • Cross-Media Marketing
    • Distribution
    • Diversification of Media Markets
    • Economies of Scale in Media Markets
    • Globalization of the Media
    • Labor in the Media
    • Labor Unions in the Media
    • Markets of the Media
    • Media Conglomerates
    • Media Management
    • Media Marketing
    • Mergers
    • Ownership in the Media
    • Piracy
    • Political Economy of the Media
    • Privatization of the Media
    • Forms of Media Corporations
    • Public Goods
    • Agenda-Setting Effects
    • Appraisal Theory
    • Media Effects on Attitudes, Values, and Beliefs
    • Cognitive Availability
    • Albert Bandura
    • Catharsis Theory
    • Steven H. Chaffee
    • Credibility Effects
    • Cumulative Media Effects
    • Desensitization
    • Diffusion of Information and Innovation
    • Emotional Arousal Theory
    • Media Effects on Emotions
    • Effects of Entertainment
    • Fear Induction through Media Content
    • Leon Festinger
    • Framing Effects
    • Frustration Aggression Theory
    • George Gerbner
    • Carl I. Hovland
    • Intercultural Media Effects
    • Elihu Katz
    • Knowledge Gap Effects
    • Latitude of Acceptance
    • Linear and Nonlinear Models of Causal Analysis
    • Mainstreaming
    • Media Effects: Direct and Indirect Effects
    • Media Effects Duration
    • History of Media Effects
    • Media Effects Models: Elaborated Models
    • Strength of Media Effects
    • Media System Dependency Theory
    • Mediating Factors
    • Mediatization of Society
    • Structure of Message Effect
    • Elisabeth Noelle-Neumann
    • Effects of Nonverbal Signals
    • Observational Learning
    • Opinion Leader
    • Order of Presentation
    • Persuasion
    • Physical Effects of Media Content
    • Priming Theory
    • Media Effects on Public Opinion
    • Reciprocal Effects
    • Schemas and Media Effects
    • Effects of Sex and Pornography as Media Content
    • Sleeper Effect
    • Media Effects on Social Behavior
    • Media Effects on Social Capital
    • Social Judgment Theory
    • Trap Effect
    • Two-Step Flow of Communication
    • Secondary Victimization
    • Effects of Violence as Media Content
    • Academy Awards
    • History of Advertising
    • BBC
    • Cable Television
    • History of Censorship
    • History of Cinematography
    • History of Citizen Journalism
    • Civil Rights Movement and the Media
    • Coffee Houses as Public Sphere
    • Effects of Violence as Media Content
    • Academy Awards
    • Collective Memory and the Media
    • History of Digital Media
    • History of Documentary Film
    • History of Elections and Media
    • Electronic Mail
    • Federal Communications Commission (FCC)
    • Fleet Street
    • Fourth Estate
    • Graffiti
    • Historic Key Events and the Media
    • Illustrated Newspapers
    • Literary Journalism
    • History of Magazine
    • Music Videos
    • Nineteenth-Century New Journalism
    • History of News Agencies
    • History of News Magazine
    • Newscast
    • 24-Hour Newscast
    • Antecedents of Newspaper
    • History of Newspaper
    • Paperback Fiction
    • Penny Press
    • History of Postal Service
    • History of Printing
    • Newsreel
    • Freedom of Communication
    • Propaganda in World War II
    • History of Public Broadcasting
    • Radical Media
    • Radio Networks
    • Radio: Social History
    • Radio Technology
    • Satellite Television
    • History of Sports and the Media
    • History of Telegraph
    • Television Networks
    • Television: Social History
    • Television Technology
    • Underground Press
    • History of Violence and the Media
    • Virtual Reality
    • Watergate Scandal
    • Women’s Movement and the Media
    • Accountability of the Media
    • Accountability of the News
    • Accuracy
    • Balance
    • Bias in the News
    • Commentary
    • Commercialization: Impact on Media Content
    • Conflict as Media Content
    • Consonance of Media Content
    • Construction of Reality through the News
    • Credibility of Content
    • Crime Reporting
    • Editorial
    • Endorsement
    • Ethics of Media Content
    • Fairness Doctrine
    • Fictional Media Content
    • Framing of the News
    • Infotainment
    • Instrumental Actualization
    • Internet
    • Internet News
    • Local News
    • Magazine
    • Media Performance
    • Morality and Taste in Media Content
    • Narrative News Story
    • Negativity
    • Neutrality
    • News
    • News Factors
    • News Production and Technology
    • News Values
    • Newspaper
    • Objectivity in Reporting
    • Plurality
    • Quality of the News
    • Quality Press
    • Radio
    • Radio News
    • Reality and Media Reality
    • Scandalization in the News
    • Sensationalism
    • Separation of News and Comments
    • Soap Operas
    • Soft News
    • Sound Bites
    • Stereotypes
    • Synchronization of the News
    • Tabloid Press
    • Tabloidization
    • Television
    • News
    • Truth and Media Content
    • Violence as Media Content
    • Africa: Media Systems
    • Austria: Media System
    • Balkan States: Media Systems
    • Baltic States: Media Systems
    • Argentina: Media System
    • Bolivia: Media System
    • Brazil: Media System
    • Canada: Media System
    • Caribbean States: Media Systems
    • Central America: Media Systems
    • Chile: Media System
    • China: Media System
    • Colombia: Media System
    • Convergence of Media Systems
    • Cuba: Media System
    • Czech Republic: Media System
    • Egypt: Media System
    • France: Media System
    • Germany: Media System
    • Gulf States: Media Systems
    • India: Media System
    • Iran: Media System
    • Israel: Media System
    • Italy: Media System
    • Japan: Media System
    • Malaysia: Media System
    • Mexico: Media System
    • Netherlands: Media System
    • North Africa: Media Systems
    • Poland: Media System
    • Portugal: Media System
    • Public Broadcasting Systems
    • Russia: Media System
    • Scandinavian States: Media Systems
    • Singapore: Media System
    • South Africa: Media System
    • South Korea: Media System
    • Spain: Media System
    • Switzerland: Media System
    • United Kingdom: Media System
    • United States of America: Media System
    • West Asia: Media Systems
    • Behavioral Norms: Perception through the Media
    • Body Images in the Media
    • Computer Games and Reality Perception
    • Cultivation Effects
    • Disowning Projection
    • Entertainment Content and Reality Perception
    • Extra-Media Data
    • False Consensus
    • False Uniqueness
    • Media Campaigns And Perceptions Of Reality
    • Media Content and Social Networks
    • Media Messages and Family Communication
    • Media and Perceptions of Reality
    • Perceived Realism as a Decision Process
    • Perceived Reality as a Communication Process
    • Perceived Reality: Meta-Analyses
    • Perceived Reality as a Social Process
    • Pluralistic Ignorance
    • Pluralistic Ignorance and Ideological Biases
    • Social Perception
    • Social Perception: Impersonal Impact
    • Social Perception: Unrealistic Optimism
    • Socialization by the Media
    • Spiral of Silence
    • Stereotyping and the Media
    • Third-Person Effects
    • Video Malaise

Custom Writing Services

Custom Writing Services