• Custom Writing Services
  • How to Write a Research Paper
  • Research Paper Topics
  • Research Paper Examples
  • Order

Communication

iResearchNet

Custom Writing Services

Custom Writing Services
Communication » Media » Cognitive Availability

Cognitive Availability




The term “availability heuristic” refers to a judgmental rule of thumb for estimating frequencies and probabilities. It states that individuals determine frequencies and probabilities “by the ease with which relevant instances come to mind” (Tversky & Kahneman 1973, 207). The logic underlying the availability heuristic holds that frequent and probable events are well represented in memory and are therefore easy to retrieve. Reversing this link, the experienced ease associated with retrieving specific events from memory can be used as an indicator for the frequency and probability of events. For example, when asked how many different game shows are currently aired on TV, one may determine how easy or difficult pertaining instances can be recalled and base the judgment on the experienced ease or difficulty associated with this recall (“If it so easy to recall examples of different game shows, then there must be many”).

The availability heuristic generally yields accurate results since frequent events are better represented in memory than infrequent ones. However, when experiences of ease or difficulty are influenced by factors other than frequency of prior exposure, the availability heuristic may be misleading. In particular, events are also more easily recalled the more recently the event occurred or was otherwise activated (recency effect), the more attention they received during encoding (salience effect), or the more the situational context of the retrieval matches the initial encoding context (congruency effect). Consequently, despite being generally accurate, the availability heuristic may produce misleading results too. As a prominent example, it has been demonstrated that individuals overestimate the prevalence of extreme causes of death, e.g., flood, homicide, tornado (Combs & Slovic 1979). The striking bias was, obviously, not a result of “reality,” but was rather a consequence of media coverage, which, it has been suggested, tends to over-report sensational events. Overreporting is likely to render extreme events very accessible, thus increasing the ease with which related pieces of information can be brought to mind. Therefore, whenever media coverage does not reflect true variation in the environment (e.g., because of overreporting of extreme events), frequency judgments are likely to be biased due to the judgmental implications of the availability heuristic.




The above examples suggest that individuals may rely on the ease or difficulty associated with retrieving information from memory when forming frequency and probability judgments. In a classic study, Tversky and Kahneman (1973) demonstrated this by asking participants whether there are more English words that begin with the letter r than those that have the letter r in the third position. Usually, individuals judge words that begin with the letter r to be more frequent those with r in the third position, despite the reverse being true. Presumably, this is because it is easier to recall words that begin with a particular letter than those in with the letter in a particular position, and the experienced ease of retrieval signals that there are many words of this kind. Although numerous studies employing different tasks and settings have supported these results, a closer look at the supporting evidence raises the questions of whether the effects of the availability heuristic are indeed mediated by experiences of ease or difficulty. On the one hand, words that begin with r are easier to recall; on the other hand, however, individuals are also likely to recall more words of this category. While the first explanation rests on the experienced ease as initially hypothesized by Tversky and Kahneman, the second explanation rests on the content information that is recalled. Addressing this confusion, Schwarz et al. (1991) developed a paradigm that allows for disentangling the competing explanations of ease versus content. Participants recalled either six or 12 instances of prior self-assertive behavior and subsequently judged their own self-assertiveness. As recalling six instances is easy, while recalling 12 is difficult, the implications of experienced ease versus recalled content information were diametrically opposed. In particular, if participants relied on their experiences of retrieval ease, they would judge their own self-assertiveness more positively after recalling six (easy) rather than 12 (difficult) instances – after all, if it is easy to come up with instances, one has to be self-assertive (and vice versa for experiences of difficulty). Contrarily, if participants relied on the content information, they would judge their own self-assertiveness more positively after recalling many rather than few instances, because more material was retrieved from memory. Judgmental results were in accordance with the easebut not the content-explanation, thus underscoring that the effects of the availability heuristic are indeed mediated by experiences of ease or difficulty.

Three recent developments deserve particular mention. For one, it has been demonstrated that not only judgments of frequency and probability, but also evaluative judgments of many kinds may be affected by experiences of ease or difficulty. For instance, judgments about consumer products, about health-related issues, or about other individuals may be informed by the ease with which relevant pieces of information come to mind.

In a more loose relation to the original idea that ease of retrieval influences judgments of frequency and probability, one could speculate that individuals may draw inferences about the importance or adequacy of a given topic for the present situation. If this were the case, issues that are strongly covered in the media – and are thus easily accessible – would be considered important because they come to mind so easily. Such a mechanism could account for the agenda-setting function of the media, besides a more traditional mere priming approach.

Moreover, one may ask how often or when individuals rely on the experienced ease as opposed to the recalled content? First, experiences of retrieval ease are likely to be relied on in conditions of low processing motivation/capacity, that is, when individuals either do not want to or cannot invest much cognitive resources in the formation of judgments and decisions. Second, experiences of retrieval ease are likely to be relied on if individuals are in a positive (rather than negative) mood state, or if individuals are low (rather than high) in depressive symptomatology. Third, experiences of retrieval ease are likely to be relied on if individuals currently or dispositionally experience uncertainty. Finally, experiences of retrieval ease are likely to be relied on only if they are perceived to be diagnostic for the judgment in question.

References:

  1. Combs, B., & Slovic, P. (1979). Newpaper coverage of causes of death. Journalism Quarterly, 56(4), 837– 843.
  2. Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 332 –348.
  3. Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 195 –202.
  4. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232.




Communication Research

Communication Research

  • Media
    • Media Economics
    • Media Effects
    • Media History
    • Media Production and Content
    • Media Systems
    • Media and Perceptions of Reality
    • Excitation Transfer Theory
    • Effects Of Exemplification And Exemplars
    • Economics of Advertising
    • Antitrust Regulation
    • Audience Commodity
    • Brands
    • Circulation
    • Commercialization of the Media
    • Competition in Media Systems
    • Concentration in Media Systems
    • Consolidation of Media Markets
    • Consumers in Media Markets
    • Cost and Revenue Structures in the Media
    • Cross-Media Marketing
    • Distribution
    • Diversification of Media Markets
    • Economies of Scale in Media Markets
    • Globalization of the Media
    • Labor in the Media
    • Labor Unions in the Media
    • Markets of the Media
    • Media Conglomerates
    • Media Management
    • Media Marketing
    • Mergers
    • Ownership in the Media
    • Piracy
    • Political Economy of the Media
    • Privatization of the Media
    • Forms of Media Corporations
    • Public Goods
    • Agenda-Setting Effects
    • Appraisal Theory
    • Media Effects on Attitudes, Values, and Beliefs
    • Cognitive Availability
    • Albert Bandura
    • Catharsis Theory
    • Steven H. Chaffee
    • Credibility Effects
    • Cumulative Media Effects
    • Desensitization
    • Diffusion of Information and Innovation
    • Emotional Arousal Theory
    • Media Effects on Emotions
    • Effects of Entertainment
    • Fear Induction through Media Content
    • Leon Festinger
    • Framing Effects
    • Frustration Aggression Theory
    • George Gerbner
    • Carl I. Hovland
    • Intercultural Media Effects
    • Elihu Katz
    • Knowledge Gap Effects
    • Latitude of Acceptance
    • Linear and Nonlinear Models of Causal Analysis
    • Mainstreaming
    • Media Effects: Direct and Indirect Effects
    • Media Effects Duration
    • History of Media Effects
    • Media Effects Models: Elaborated Models
    • Strength of Media Effects
    • Media System Dependency Theory
    • Mediating Factors
    • Mediatization of Society
    • Structure of Message Effect
    • Elisabeth Noelle-Neumann
    • Effects of Nonverbal Signals
    • Observational Learning
    • Opinion Leader
    • Order of Presentation
    • Persuasion
    • Physical Effects of Media Content
    • Priming Theory
    • Media Effects on Public Opinion
    • Reciprocal Effects
    • Schemas and Media Effects
    • Effects of Sex and Pornography as Media Content
    • Sleeper Effect
    • Media Effects on Social Behavior
    • Media Effects on Social Capital
    • Social Judgment Theory
    • Trap Effect
    • Two-Step Flow of Communication
    • Secondary Victimization
    • Effects of Violence as Media Content
    • Academy Awards
    • History of Advertising
    • BBC
    • Cable Television
    • History of Censorship
    • History of Cinematography
    • History of Citizen Journalism
    • Civil Rights Movement and the Media
    • Coffee Houses as Public Sphere
    • Effects of Violence as Media Content
    • Academy Awards
    • Collective Memory and the Media
    • History of Digital Media
    • History of Documentary Film
    • History of Elections and Media
    • Electronic Mail
    • Federal Communications Commission (FCC)
    • Fleet Street
    • Fourth Estate
    • Graffiti
    • Historic Key Events and the Media
    • Illustrated Newspapers
    • Literary Journalism
    • History of Magazine
    • Music Videos
    • Nineteenth-Century New Journalism
    • History of News Agencies
    • History of News Magazine
    • Newscast
    • 24-Hour Newscast
    • Antecedents of Newspaper
    • History of Newspaper
    • Paperback Fiction
    • Penny Press
    • History of Postal Service
    • History of Printing
    • Newsreel
    • Freedom of Communication
    • Propaganda in World War II
    • History of Public Broadcasting
    • Radical Media
    • Radio Networks
    • Radio: Social History
    • Radio Technology
    • Satellite Television
    • History of Sports and the Media
    • History of Telegraph
    • Television Networks
    • Television: Social History
    • Television Technology
    • Underground Press
    • History of Violence and the Media
    • Virtual Reality
    • Watergate Scandal
    • Women’s Movement and the Media
    • Accountability of the Media
    • Accountability of the News
    • Accuracy
    • Balance
    • Bias in the News
    • Commentary
    • Commercialization: Impact on Media Content
    • Conflict as Media Content
    • Consonance of Media Content
    • Construction of Reality through the News
    • Credibility of Content
    • Crime Reporting
    • Editorial
    • Endorsement
    • Ethics of Media Content
    • Fairness Doctrine
    • Fictional Media Content
    • Framing of the News
    • Infotainment
    • Instrumental Actualization
    • Internet
    • Internet News
    • Local News
    • Magazine
    • Media Performance
    • Morality and Taste in Media Content
    • Narrative News Story
    • Negativity
    • Neutrality
    • News
    • News Factors
    • News Production and Technology
    • News Values
    • Newspaper
    • Objectivity in Reporting
    • Plurality
    • Quality of the News
    • Quality Press
    • Radio
    • Radio News
    • Reality and Media Reality
    • Scandalization in the News
    • Sensationalism
    • Separation of News and Comments
    • Soap Operas
    • Soft News
    • Sound Bites
    • Stereotypes
    • Synchronization of the News
    • Tabloid Press
    • Tabloidization
    • Television
    • News
    • Truth and Media Content
    • Violence as Media Content
    • Africa: Media Systems
    • Austria: Media System
    • Balkan States: Media Systems
    • Baltic States: Media Systems
    • Argentina: Media System
    • Bolivia: Media System
    • Brazil: Media System
    • Canada: Media System
    • Caribbean States: Media Systems
    • Central America: Media Systems
    • Chile: Media System
    • China: Media System
    • Colombia: Media System
    • Convergence of Media Systems
    • Cuba: Media System
    • Czech Republic: Media System
    • Egypt: Media System
    • France: Media System
    • Germany: Media System
    • Gulf States: Media Systems
    • India: Media System
    • Iran: Media System
    • Israel: Media System
    • Italy: Media System
    • Japan: Media System
    • Malaysia: Media System
    • Mexico: Media System
    • Netherlands: Media System
    • North Africa: Media Systems
    • Poland: Media System
    • Portugal: Media System
    • Public Broadcasting Systems
    • Russia: Media System
    • Scandinavian States: Media Systems
    • Singapore: Media System
    • South Africa: Media System
    • South Korea: Media System
    • Spain: Media System
    • Switzerland: Media System
    • United Kingdom: Media System
    • United States of America: Media System
    • West Asia: Media Systems
    • Behavioral Norms: Perception through the Media
    • Body Images in the Media
    • Computer Games and Reality Perception
    • Cultivation Effects
    • Disowning Projection
    • Entertainment Content and Reality Perception
    • Extra-Media Data
    • False Consensus
    • False Uniqueness
    • Media Campaigns And Perceptions Of Reality
    • Media Content and Social Networks
    • Media Messages and Family Communication
    • Media and Perceptions of Reality
    • Perceived Realism as a Decision Process
    • Perceived Reality as a Communication Process
    • Perceived Reality: Meta-Analyses
    • Perceived Reality as a Social Process
    • Pluralistic Ignorance
    • Pluralistic Ignorance and Ideological Biases
    • Social Perception
    • Social Perception: Impersonal Impact
    • Social Perception: Unrealistic Optimism
    • Socialization by the Media
    • Spiral of Silence
    • Stereotyping and the Media
    • Third-Person Effects
    • Video Malaise

Custom Writing Services

Custom Writing Services